Antidepressant withdrawal review: authors respond in detail to Mental Elf critique

In this response we will clearly evidence why Hayes and Jauhar’s blog critique of our systematic review was often factually incorrect and was replete with misrepresentations (and/or misunderstandings) that will lead some readers to conclude wrongly that our findings are not robust.

The seven points we will make are that Hayes and Jauhar:

1) allege, without justification, incompleteness in our search strategy, while it was as least as thorough as other systematic reviews in this area

2) misrepresent or simply misunderstand our exclusion/inclusion criteria

3) appear entirely unconcerned by the bias in the RCTs, while overstating the bias in the surveys

4) wrongly imply we did not declare our conflicts of interest

5) misrepresent or simply misunderstand why RCTs and naturalistic studies did not inform our severity estimates

6) allege data extraction errors we did not make

7) by raising the issue of differences between the drugs, commit the all-too-common fallacy of criticising a study for not doing what it never set out to do

Before we consider these points in turn, we would first like to explain why we are posting our response here.

On the same day as Hayes and Jauhar’s published their blog critique (18 Oct 2018), we invited them to submit it to the journal in which we published our article, Addictive Behaviors, so we could respond to each of their points in the appropriate peer-review setting. As they only submitted to the journal on 2 Nov 2018, and as the peer-review/publishing process will now take additional unspecified time, we have taken the decision to respond to their blog post here, given that there is growing appetite in the professional and service user communities for our response, and that we are anxious not to endure any further delay in setting the record straight. We have also sent this response to Mental Elf for publication.

1) Search strategy

Hayes and Jauhar (2018) allege that it is ‘highly likely’ that our search strategy did not find all relevant studies. We are confident that our strategy, which included MEDLINE/PubMed, PsycINFO, Google Scholar, previous reviews and the bibliographies of 20 relevant papers, is at least as thorough as most systematic reviews with regard to identifying published studies. We have subsequently searched for relevant unpublished theses, dissertations and conference proceedings, in ProQuest and OpenGrey, and found none.

2) Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Hayes and Jauhar point out that we did not register our inclusion criteria in advance. In response, aside from PRISMA not obliging such criteria to be preregistered (see item #5), we clearly state our eligibility criteria (which complies with PRISMA guideline item #6, while further address PRISMA items 7-10# in the methods section). Additionally, we surpassed most systematic reviews on depression treatments as only a minority (around 30% – Chung et al., 2018) include lists of included and excluded studies, as we do.

Hayes and Jauhar then suggest that we should have identified the length of follow-up, length of antidepressant exposure, and drug-company funding as reasons for exclusion before undertaking the search and data extraction. Firstly, we went beyond both what PRISMA requests and the procedure of previous systematic reviews in this area, by explicitly stating in each relevant section a rationale for excluding each of the studies omitted. Because we did so, any diligent reader will clearly see that none of the three variables that Hayes and Jauhar raise were the sole reason for excluding any study at all (hence our not identifying them as sole reasons for exclusion before our search and data extraction). For example, as the tables make clear, six of the included 24 studies were clearly identified as drug-company funded. Furthermore, the five drug-company studies excluded from our estimate of incidence, while indeed reporting artificially short durations, were excluded on the quite obvious ground that they failed to report incidence rates. This was all plainly stated, even though Hayes and Jauhar wrongly imply otherwise.

Hayes and Jauhar also take issue with our excluding two studies from our estimates of incidence “because they assessed only 9 withdrawal symptoms”. This is, once again, misleading. We did not exclude them on this basis alone but, as explicitly stated, on a variety of methodological considerations. For example, both studies were ‘chart-reviews’ of medical notes, which are notoriously weak owing to their reliance on practitioners being aware of, and recording, withdrawal reactions, while one study, oddly enough, excluded any withdrawal reactions commencing three days after discontinuation.

Finally, we note that Hayes and Jauhar only find reasons to challenge the exclusion of studies with relatively low incidence rates, but do not find fault with, or even acknowledge, our exclusion of a study with a 97% incidence rate.

3) Assessment of study quality

Hayes and Jauhar state ‘This review does not attempt a traditional assessment of bias in the studies they include’. The methodology of every one of the 24 studies is described, in text and tables, so that readers can assess for themselves their quality, including any sample biases. Furthermore, our ‘Limitations’ section acknowledges the potential minimising bias of the RCTs because of their artificially short treatment and follow-up durations (about which Hayes and Jauhar express no concern), and the possible maximising bias of the surveys because they may attract a disproportionate number of people unhappy with their drugs (about which they express grave concern). We also pointed out, however, that surveys can be prone to bias either way – e.g. one of the largest surveys included contained unusually high proportions of people who thought the drugs had helped them, so it is feasible, in this case, that the sample bias may have been towards people with a generally positive attitude about antidepressants, and therefore the study underestimated adverse effects such as withdrawal. While the RCTs had extremely artificial samples and conditions (and small numbers) the large online surveys, while not necessarily representative of all users (like the RCTs), represented the real life experiences of several thousand people with a range of treatment durations (from weeks to years) and various speeds of withdrawal.

4) Conflict of interest

Hayes and Jauhar appear to imply that we may have undisclosed ‘ideological’ conflicts of interest, something that can presumably be alleged of any researcher, author or, indeed, blogger in this area. We fully abided by the Conflict of Interest policy of the journal in which we published (Addictive Behaviors, 2018). 

5) Outcome measures

Hayes and Jauhar rightly state that in three of the incidence studies that we reviewed some withdrawal symptoms (as identified by the DESS) were also present in some of those continuing to take antidepressants. However, Hayes and Jauhar wrongly claim that in the Zajecka (1998) study withdrawal incidence is ‘higher’ in those continuing to take antidepressants compared to those stopping. The difference between the two overall rates was clearly stated in the study as not statistically different. Furthermore four specific withdrawal effects (dizziness, dysmenorrhea, rhinitis and somnolence) were significantly more common in participants who had come off the drugs. No specific effects were significantly more common in the participants who had stayed on the drugs.

6) Measuring severity

Here Hayes and Jauhar either misrepresent or simply misunderstand the reasons the RCTs and naturalistic studies did not inform our overall severity estimates. Had they read these studies carefully they would have realised that these studies did not provide any data on the severity of withdrawal effects. The one RCT that did provide severity data (Sir et al., 2005) was, as stipulated, excluded for two reasons: it only covered eight weeks treatment (which would lower severity rates), and because it was a clinician-rated rather than self-report. Hayes and Jauhar choose not to acknowledge that our review also stated that even if we had included this outlier the weighted average of people who described their withdrawal effects as severe would have reduced only slightly, from 45.7% to 43.5%. (i)

Furthermore Hayes and Jauhar misrepresented our 45.7% weighted average by falsely stating that we concluded that ‘The severity of these symptoms is severe in over half of cases’.

7) Mixing study designs (opposition to including surveys)

Hayes and Jauhar argue that it is questionable to combine data from randomised controlled trials and naturalistic studies with survey data. As RCTs and naturalistic studies are regularly covered in systematic reviews (Guyatt et al. 2008; Egger et al. 2001), they obviously object to our inclusion of surveys. In making this objection, however, Hayes and Jauhar are simply confusing a methodological preference with a methodological law. There is no law prohibiting the inclusion of experiential survey data in a systematic review. In fact, given that the design of the majority of RTCs lead to the incidence, severity and duration of withdrawal being minimised (completely failing to capture real world experience of patients), it would have been unethical to omit such experiential data. Psychiatry has too often been guilty of devaluing the importance of experiential knowledge in its evaluation of interventions, which has in turn undermined our capacity to positively intervene.

Even so, given that surveys are open to specific forms of bias, how different would the estimates have been had we omitted surveys from our analysis? The answer is found in noting that the three types of studies, when grouped, did not differ greatly in terms of withdrawal incidence. The weighted averages turn out as follows:

  • The three surveys – 57.1% (1790/3137),
  • The four naturalistic studies – 50.4% (115/228)
  • The seven RCTs – 51.4% (353/687)

As getting similar findings from different methodologies is typically seen to strengthen confidence in an overall, combined estimate, it is safe to conclude that at least half of people suffer withdrawal symptoms when trying to come off antidepressants.

Minor errors

Hayes and Jauhar report 3 very minor errors in presentation, which do not impact our estimates. Where we are concerned, however, is that they purport to identify two further ‘errors’, which are clearly not errors at all.  For example, their assertion that the ‘total number experiencing withdrawal in the study by Sir and colleagues is 83 rather than 110’ is wrong. They reached this figure by unjustifiably removing all withdrawal symptoms rated ‘minimal’, while not informing readers they did this. This unwarranted decision not only minimised the rate of incidence but also created the false impression that we were in error.

Their second error concerns their suggestion that we misrepresented the incidence rate of another study (Montgomery et al. 2005) by presenting the incidence of withdrawal following escitalopram treatment as ‘27%, when it is 16%.’ Here Hayes and Jauhar again mislead the reader. The 27% rate we reported was at one week and the 16% they reported was at two weeks. Given we were calculating for incidence, it was absolutely correct for us to use the 27% figure in our calculations. These are unfortunate errors for Hayes and Jauhar to make, which, if left uncorrected, would wrongly undermine confidence in our study for some readers.

Combining data from different types of antidepressants

Hayes and Jauhar point out that because medications with longer half-lives will be associated with ‘less’ withdrawal effects “it is puzzling that the results are presented for all antidepressants combined”. Firstly, in the paper we explicitly acknowledge that “differing half-lives affect timing of withdrawal onset”, so they are telling us nothing we don’t already declare. Furthermore, ‘combining results’, or, more accurately, advancing global estimates was both necessary and appropriate given the central aim of our review. To reiterate, our aim was to assess whether NICE guidelines (2009) on antidepressant withdrawal were evidence based, not to guide clinicians in what drugs to prescribe nor to illuminate the particularities of different pharmacokinetic properties. Here Hayes and Jauhar commit the all-too-common fallacy of criticising a study for not doing what it never set out to do.

Conclusion

For the reasons stated we believe Hayes and Jahaur’s commentary to be inaccurate and misleading overall. In some cases the critiques they offer are based on obvious misrepresentations of study findings.

We fully accept that our overall estimates of 56% incidence, with 46% of those being severe, are only estimates. They may move by a few percentage points in either direction. However, even if the actual incidence is towards the lower end of the 50% to 57% range, when grouping study types, this will still constitute over half of all antidepressant users. It is crucial that amid the complexities of academic disagreement we do not lose sight of the scale of the problem.

In the light of this, it is also interesting to note the absence of any acknowledgment that we are discussing a public health issue involving millions of people worldwide.  Hayes and Jahaur also fail to comment on the primary finding of the review, namely that national guidelines in the USA and the UK significantly misjudge the true extent of the problem.

In the sprit of seeking some common ground between us, we can agree with one concluding statement Hayes and Jahaur make:

‘It reflects negatively on the whole of the field of psychiatry that there is not better, clearer evidence from high quality studies on the incidence, severity and duration of any symptoms related to antidepressant cessation.’

Given that 16% of our adult population was prescribed an antidepressant last year alone, this professional oversight, and its significance, is hard to excuse.

While better research is indeed desirable (with respect to a diversity of issues pertaining to withdrawal), the millions experiencing withdrawal effects cannot wait for psychiatry to determine whether they represent 56%, 51% or 61% of those withdrawing, or what will be the best methodologies to assess that. They need accurate information and proper support now. And the millions more who will consider starting antidepressants in the coming years are entitled, unlike those who have gone before, to receive accurate information about all the adverse effects including the difficulty they are very likely to encounter when they try to stop; difficulties that in far too many cases will be protracted and severe. A crucial step forward will be for government bodies and professional organisations to update their guidelines so as to render them evidence-based.

Dr James Davies

Professor John Read

References

Addictive Behaviors (2018) Guide for Authors (Declaration of Interest). Website: https://www.elsevier.com/journals/addictive-behaviors/0306-4603/guide-for-authorsAccessed Oct 2018.

Chung, V.C.H., et al. (2018) Methodological quality of systematic reviews on treatments for depression: a cross-sectional study. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 27 (26): 619-627

Egger M. et al. (2001) Systematic reviews in health care: meta-analysis in context. 2nd ed. London (UK): BMJ Publishing Group.

Guyatt G. et al. (2008) Users’ guides to the medical literature. 2nd ed. New York (NY): McGraw Hill Medical.

Hayes, J. & Jauhar, S. (2018) Antidepressant withdrawal: reviewing the paper behind the headlines. Mental Elf. Website: https://www.nationalelfservice.net/treatment/antidepressants/antidepressant-withdrawal-reviewing-the-paper-behind-the-headlines/. Accessed Oct 2018.

Montgomery, S. A. et al. (2005) ‘A 24-week randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of escitalopram for the prevention of generalized social anxiety disorder’, The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66(10): 1270–1278.

The National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) (2009) NICE Depression in adults: recognition and management. Website https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/cg90/resources/depression-in-adults-recognition-and-management-pdf-975742638037, Accessed Jul 2018.

Price, J. et al. (1996) A comparison of the post-marketing safety of four selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors including the investigation of symptoms occurring on withdrawal, British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 42: 757–763.

Rosenbaum, J. F. et al. (1998) Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor discontinuation syndrome: a randomized clinical trial, Biological Psychiatry, 44(2): 77–87.

Zajecka, J. et al. (1998) Safety of Abrupt Discontinuation of Fluoxetine: A Randomized, Placebo-Controlled Study, Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 18(3): 193.

____________

(i) Hayes and Jauhar may have confused the severity data in Rosenbaum et al. (1998) for data on withdrawal severity. To be clear, such severity data pertained to depression severity pre- and post-discontinuation not withdrawal severity. Secondly, Price et al. (1996) indicate that 79% of withdrawal reactions were rated either ‘moderately severe or severe’. As Price et al. offer no scales or definitions by which to assess precisely what ‘moderately severe or severe’ means, we had no way of confidently placing their data (e.g. was ‘severe’ divided into three categories: ‘mildly severe’, ‘moderately severe’ and ‘severe’?  If their data were included in our systematic review on the basis of such assumed divisions, then our overall severity estimates would have been higher).

 

 

 

14 Responses to Antidepressant withdrawal review: authors respond in detail to Mental Elf critique

  1. Jan Waterton 05/11/2018 at 8:46 am #

    I totally agree with the final paragraph. I have been off Clonazepam for chronic pain and still have the aftermath withdrawal problems ie pins and needles, electric shocks, muscle spasms 8 years after stopping. I have a very busy happy life , but some days when it’s very bad, it’s very frustrating, when I read some reports these from these DKs saying these don’t exists makes me so angry. I have just had to see a DK regarding something and for the first time ever, actually knew about the long term damage these Benzos and Anti depressant can cause , he and his wife who is also a GP, never prescribe Benzos, it was such a joy to have found someone I could talk to, but mainly understood. But sadly is now off for the 3 months travelling at the start of his retirement.

  2. christine bell 05/11/2018 at 12:26 pm #

    I have been off seroxat for 12 years and I am still having stomach problems, cant take any medication, muscle aches, head shocks, tinnitus, I could go on and on, I came off benzos after 15 years and never had the problems I now have with coming off seroxat so there is something different in a/ds to cause so many people so many problems. I was put on them for migraines, I would trade migraines for what I have now, but I still have migraines, although they were not as painful on seroxat. I got to the stage where I was needing to up the seroxat, but of course they are not addictive!!!

  3. Ruth Francis 05/11/2018 at 3:40 pm #

    I Stopped Seroxat 13 years ago after taking them for 8 years .My Doctor never mentioned any possible Withdrawl problems . The withdrawl has been unbelievable I have suffered from extreme anxiety, agitation, really bad depression , dizziness and balance problems, stomach problems, extreme head aches , electric shocks in head mad I lost 2 and a half stone in weight. I had no help from my doctor whatsoever I am surprised I am still here to be honest and 13 years later I still have balance issues and dizziness, stomach problems and headaches and I am so sensitive to medication I cannot take anything now. My initial problem was panic attacks in busy places but at least I had a reasonable quality of life unlike now. I am so grateful for all concerned for bringing this awful problem into the public domain.

    • kiwi 05/11/2018 at 8:31 pm #

      Thankyou Ruth for posting. You story is sooo common.
      What is outrageously shameful is its all denied by doctors.

      “I had no help from my doctor whatsoever I am surprised I am still here to be honest ”

      This is exactly my testimony too.

      In fact i had doctors (6 in number) mock me, insult me, diagnonsense me, offend me, refer me, talk blithering nonsense to me and raise their voice against me when i decided to make official documented complaints. As well as thump their fist on the desk. I invited several of them to try some of their own medicine their responding body language and refusal said it all.

      The sad thing is after reading of thousands of other stories it has become obvious to me that our experience is the norm not the exception.

  4. OB 05/11/2018 at 7:51 pm #

    My long experience with Psychiatrist, psychintrist nurses and records has show me that most of the time , comments, complaints etc made by the patients in relation to physical symptoms are most of the time ignored and in best scenarios distorted.
    If they were taken in consideration or recorded most of psychiatrist may be the case of having to face criminal charges for negligence and harm. That is why most probably are ignored.

  5. kiwi 05/11/2018 at 8:33 pm #

    Thankyou Dr Davies and Dr Read for pushing back agaisnt these clowns.

  6. kiwi 05/11/2018 at 9:18 pm #

    “Given that 16% of our adult population was prescribed an antidepressant last year alone, this professional oversight, and its significance, is hard to excuse.”

    Wow we are talking massive numbers harmed here. Its in the many millions!

    The population from google of the uk is approx 66 million.
    Googling to find the adult (above 18?) population numbers i get very roughly (it appears from the mid 2017 dataset that there is about (conservatively eyeballing it ) 730,000 per each age on average under 18 including 0 age which gives a total of about 13 million.

    So total adult poplution must be about 53 million.

    16% of this is 8.5 million!!

    The numbers of course are bigger than this as we know full well that despite black box warnings children and teenagers are given these horrific chemicals.

    Apparently it is alleged someone on social media suggested doctors pushing these drugs are worse than Hitler. I can understand why such comments are made. Talk about a covert increasingly successful final solution. Some might argue that ssri withdrawal is a torture worse than death. Many choose this option rather than continue the withdrawal struggle.

    Perhaps the hypocratic oath should be similar to a quote from Hitlers “Nature is cruel, therefore we also are entitled to be cruel…”

    On instructions from the Pharma/ [Fuhrer]- I make known the following:
    Where the withdrawal Question is brought up in public, there may be no discussion ….
    It may, however, be mentioned that the ssri-ers are taken in DSM groups for appropriate diagnostical purposes.
    Signed M. Bormann

  7. Professor David Clark 06/11/2018 at 7:01 am #

    Well done James and John for your excellent response.

  8. Terry 06/11/2018 at 5:46 pm #

    I have been on the antidepressant merry go round for over 20 years for something that could have been avoided when was younger the first antidepressant I took made me suicidel but was kept on it after 6 months it was stopped I now know I had withdrawels and so another one was added and so on firstly I had a chemical imbalance and I needed these meds for life even though no history of depression before them then 5 years ago something happened to me and I began to become very sick I had become tolerant to the drug I was going into withdrawel despite taking it for the next 3 years I was placed on over 10 different medications to combat the symptoms all the time being told that it was anxiety and depression then 21 months ago all meds were stopped the last 5 years of my life have been nothing but torture the insomnia and extreme burning of my whole body the inner tremors the sweating and heat intolerance without the severe crushing depression no one should ever have to go through then the next set of lies from the doctors it’s all in my head I’m a liar we need to find the right meds , I have been laughed at called stupid and one doctor told me that I needed a good kick up the backside by the doctors that I trusted now I go to them for help and I get nothing I am told that there is no more we can do for you .

    The next thing is not one of the doctors I have visited have acknowledged protracted withdrawel many have denied it say these drugs are harmless and are safe but then they say it can’t be withdrawel still as you have been of the drugs for so long they are out of your system so there you are they tell you that there is no such thing then next there is but it can’t be now . For Hayes and jauhar to make such bold and inaccurate statements they need to look more closely on the internet and read through the many groups of tens of thousands of people who’s lives have been severely destroyed by the wrong information and the lack of knowledge from the prescribers who administer these harmful drugs the pharmaceutical company’s sell push these drugs on doctors with all the wrong information making out how safe they are without having to enclose all the real details of the trials and harms we as patient are led to believe that these drugs are cures and are safe but when they go bad we are blamed it is so easy for these critics to judge us when they are not harmed they should live our lives for a week and see if they then feel the same , phyciatrist and pharma should hang their heads in shame

  9. margaret Braithwaite 08/11/2018 at 8:56 am #

    I am a F/T care of my husband who is now med free after being on medication for 29 years The drug that has done the damage is lorazepam which he was prescribed for 2and a half years . It took me over 2 years to taper Phil off after a reinstatement and then a further 5 months to taper off pregabilin which finished 8 months ago . He has resurgences of the symptoms eg prickly skin ,and eyes ,felling intolerably hot ,gastric pains which are ongoing ,leg pains ,high anxiety and sensitivity to cold . Recently because I had an eye op Phil went into an NHS respite jhospital where he met a very understanding doctor who had worked there for over 30 years and had come accross many patients suffering from benzo withdrawal -he admitted he could do nothing for them and wouldn`t allow any benzos in the hospital . Phil has lost a lot of weight and looks as if he`e been in a japanese war camp but I am not giving up and believe like Una Corbett of BAT and Ian Singleton of Bristol Tranquilliser Support Group that Phil will get better . To me he has been chemically poisoned and his gaba receptors made dependant -now they must work for themselves and this to me accounts for the withdrawal symptoms ,these receptors control the nervous system . For so called doctors to not recognise the effects of these evil drugs beggars belief . The hippocratic oat is about doing no harm yet when these drug are given out they do . What is needed is psychologists who can help patients understand their grief or upset.

  10. Altostrata 11/02/2019 at 8:40 pm #

    My not yet published comment on the Mental Elf article:

    Regarding the position of Drs. Jauhar and Hayes in their recent paper “The War on Antidepressants”, based on the above article:

    It is bizarre that a spotlight on a potentially serious adverse effect of a class of drugs is cast as a war on the drugs themselves.

    The assumption that antidepressant withdrawal symptoms generally are mild, transitory, and last only a few weeks was promulgated in a pair of supplements to the Journal of Clinical Psychiatry in 1997 and 2006 arising from “expert” symposia sponsored by pharmaceutical manufacturers Lilly and Wyeth, respectively, and led by the notorious Dr. Alan Schatzberg.

    The conclusions of the “consensus panel” were based only on the opinions of the participants. There was no data or real evidence involved. No citations were given for the statements about the severity of withdrawal syndrome.

    These papers are buried in the citations of nearly all other medical literature about antidepressant withdrawal syndrome, with the erroneous assumptions circulated over and over until they calcified throughout psychiatry into “evidence.”

    To his credit, one of the experts from Schatzberg’s “consensus panel,” the UK’s Dr. Peter Haddad, repeatedly has made an effort to remedy this misinformation, authoring many papers about withdrawal syndrome and warning about its misdiagnosis. He has pointed out repeatedly that withdrawal symptoms may be relatively mild only in most cases — there are exceptions, the extent of which is unknown.

    As Dr. Haddad stated in 2001: “Discontinuation symptoms have received little systematic study with the result that most of the recommendations made here are based on anecdotal data or expert opinion.” (Haddad, P.M. Drug-Safety (2001) 24: 183. https://doi.org/10.2165/00002018-200124030-00003)

    I also had personal correspondence in 2006 with another member of the expert panel, Dr. Richard Shelton, who admitted to me that some individuals can suffer severe and prolonged withdrawal syndrome. (Like Dr. Schatzberg, Dr. Shelton went on to a lucrative career as a pharmaceutical company consultant.)

    The “experts” who presented their opinions as evidence informing medicine’s assumptions about psychiatric drug withdrawal are well aware they have not disclosed all the risks. Consequently, physicians everywhere have a false sense of safety about these drugs and are blind to the adverse effects.

    However, given the extremely high rate of psychiatric prescription, the expedient gloss over the potential of injury has caused damage to millions of people.

    In correspondence years ago with Dr. Haddad, he hinted that gathering case histories would be instructive in this debate. On my Web site, SurvivingAntidepressants.org, I have gathered almost 6,000 case histories of difficult psychiatric drug withdrawal, none of them mild, transient, and lasting a few weeks. You can see them here http://survivingantidepressants.org/index.php?/forum/3-introductions-and-updates/

    These case histories also demonstrate the many, many ways people are being misdiagnosed and misprescribed. Taken together, they’re a landscape of the pitfalls in medical knowledge regarding psychiatric drugs and their adverse effects.

    Another critic of the Read and Davies paper, Dr. Ronald Pies, recently contended in Psychiatric Times (https://www.psychiatrictimes.com/couch-crisis/sorting-out-antidepressant-withdrawal-controversy and comments) that psychiatrists know how to taper people slowly off drugs and therefore serious withdrawal syndrome as reported by Read and Davies is nearly non-existent.

    Dr. Pies’s claims are based solely on his own 2012 paper, in its prolix entirety at https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3398684/, in which he states:

    “In my own practice, I would typically “wean” a patient off a chronically administered antidepressant over a period of 3 to 6 months and sometimes longer. To my knowledge, this period of tapering has rarely, if ever, been used in existing studies of antidepressants or in routine clinical practice.”

    Dr. Pies knows very well that “proper psychiatric care” for tapering “managed appropriately” is virtually impossible for patients to find. This tends to confirm Davies and Read are on the right track.

    Longer taper periods would definitely be better than the haphazard ways physicians, psychiatrists included, are tapering people now. Calling preliminary investigation into a common adverse reaction a “war on antidepressants” does nothing to advance patient safety. Drs. Read and Davies are correct, we need better ways to taper people off psychiatric drugs.

  11. Helen 23/04/2019 at 10:38 am #

    I have been taking Citalopram 40mg per day for so many years I can’t even remember. I’ve also had several long periods of psychology treatment and private counselling. My GP is generally lovely, but on her advice I have tried to come off the tablets twice by progressively halving my dose. The results were disastrous. My anxiety went through the roof both times. I would wake up each morning terrified of… absolutely nothing, but terrified constantly all the same. It was a living nightmare. I tried to go through it hoping I’d come out the other side, but gave in when I became suicidal, and had to go back on 40mg per day again. I think the work of Davies and Read is very important, and their rebuttal of the Mental Elf critique. (Who is behind Mental Elf? Big pharma?) Davies and Read’s work, and the public discussion it has generated, has helped me understand much slower methods of trying to wean off antidepressants which I can discuss with my GP, especially trying to get them in liquid form for more gradual reduction. But also recognise that maybe I’ll never be able to come off my high dose.

Trackbacks/Pingbacks

  1. Tumbling Further Down the Rabbit Hole: the Disturbing World of Antidepressant Withdrawal Research - - 08/01/2019

    […] Before we get to the essential part, let us first recall that our systematic review in Addictive Behaviors (2018) showed, among other things, that around half of people who stop antidepressants experience withdrawal. This conclusion was critiqued in a blog by Joseph Hayes and Sameer Jauhar, to which we responded by pointing out the blog’s many serious errors and misrepresentations (see our response here: http://cepuk.org/2018/11/05/antidepressant-withdrawal-review-authors-respond-mental-elf-critique/ […]

  2. Descendo mais no buraco de coelho da pesquisa sobre abstinência de antidepressivos | Mad In Brasil - 06/02/2019

    […] Antes de chegarmos à parte essencial, vamos primeiro lembrar que a nossa revisão sistemática apresentada em Addictive Behaviors (2018) mostrou, entre outras coisas, que cerca da metade das pessoas que param de tomar antidepressivos experimentam sintomas da abstinência. Essa conclusão foi criticada em um blog de Joseph Hayes e Sameer Jauhar, ao qual respondemos apontando os muitos erros e deturpações graves do blog (vejam a nossa resposta aqui). […]

Leave a Reply to Jan Waterton Click here to cancel reply.

Powered by WordPress. Designed by WooThemes